Educating Gifted Students for the Future
By James H. Borland, Ph.D.(注1)
Teachers College, Columbia University
ABSTRACT
In this presentation, I will focus on problems, issues, and practices related
to differentiating curriculu m for gifted students with the goal of helping these
students develop their potential and achieve success in college and professional
life. I will start with a brief discussion of the various ways in which giftedness
is conceived and operationalized and the implications of these conceptions for
curriculum development and differentiation. I will then discuss crucial issues
in curriculum differentiation for gifted students and the practical implications
of each of these choices. I will also make recommendations for future practice.
The Problem of Giftedness
It may strike one as odd to see a paper by a professor of education whose area
of study is the education of gifted students begin with the heading, "The
Problem of Giftedness." After all, giftedness is usually considered to
be a strength, a blessing, a resource-anything but a problem. However, for people
who study such things, giftedness is, conceptually and with respect to educational
practice, a problem. Perhaps that is a good working definition of a scholar:
someone who can turn a giftsintosa problem. Nonetheless, like many things, the
more one subjects the concept of giftedness, especially as it manifests itself
in the idea of gifted students in elementary and secondary schools, to disciplined
study, the more problematic it becomes. This is not merely a concern for academics.
Problems and issues related to the concept of gifted students, in the United
States at least, have serious practical implications that are of major concern
for children, parents, teachers, and school officials.
Central to the problem of giftedness is its relative, contingent nature. As
I and many others have argued, giftedness is a social construction, not a fact
of nature. It is something that was created, not a pre-existing thing that was
discovered. Its meaning derives from the discourse of educators, policy makers,
and scholars, not from the steady accretion of empirical data generated through
disciplined inquiry. In the United States, gifted children were more or less
invented in the second decade of the twentieth century in response to observed
individual differences among school children in academic achievement and especially
in scores on IQ tests, whose widespread and enthusiastic adoption by educators
was seen as reflecting a new era of progressive, effective, scientific schooling.
That children differ along a number of dimensions is undeniable, and that many
of these have implications for the education is also true. However, the particular
way in which educators responded to the fact of individual differences, which
established the norm for special education and gifted education in the U.S.
that prevails even today, was not an inevitable response, at least from a scientific
point of view. Educators could have seen diversity and variability among school
children as a normal, natural, even desirable fact of life and organized schools
and developed curriculum in a way that reflected this belief. Instead, they
resorted to a series of rather crude classifications based on the normal distribution
of IQs that divided childrensintosdistinct groups-using first the terms "subnormal,"
"normal," and "supernormal"; later "retarded,"
"normal," and "gifted."
To repeat what I think is an important point, the creation of the category
of gifted children (and the corresponding category of retarded children with
seemingly scientific fine distinctions made between "imbeciles," "morons,"
and "idiots," again based on IQ) was not empirically or logically
necessary. It is much better understood in light of social, political, and educational
policy forces and the historical fact that the schools were seen as a tool for
assimilating massive numbers of immigrant childrensintosa society dominated
by white Protestant values and as a tool for providing a workforce for an industrial
nation just beginning to dominate the world's economy.
For whatever reasons, early in the twentieth century, gifted children started
appearing in U.S. schools, and the field of gifted education emerged as a branch
of educational research, theory, and practice. But the fact that giftedness
among children was a social construction, something derived from social and
historical rather than scientific forces, made educational practice difficult.
Chief among the difficulties was the fact that as time wore on, giftedness began
to mean different things to different people and, as a consequence of the radically
decentralized structure of American education, was defined differently from
state to state, school to school.
At first, educators thought of giftedness solely in terms of IQ test scores,
since, when the tests were first introduced, most educators seemed to accept
uncritically the idea that intelligence was a unitary quantity that varied from
individual to individual, a quantity that could be measured accurately and scientifically.
Even then, however, the proper IQ cut-off for identifying giftedness was a matter
of dispute. Lewis M. Terman of Stanford University, often called the "father
of gifted education in the United States," used a cut-off of 140 on his
test, the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, to define giftedness in the work
he began in the 1920s. Terman's contemporary, Leta Hollingworth, of Teachers
College, Columbia University, used a Stanford-Binet cut-off of 180, a score
theoretically attained by only one person in one million, in her studies of
giftedness. Schools often used much lower cut-offs, with 120 being a common
score used to distinguish between gifted students and the rest.
Even more disruptive to consensus regarding the meaning of giftedness was a
series of challenges to the idea that intelligence was unitary, quantitative,
and fixed and that IQ tests were valid measures of intelligence. Alternative
conceptions of human abilities proliferated in the middle decades of the twentieth
century, and this caused a corresponding proliferation of alternative definitions
of giftedness. The widespread use of the phrase "gifted and talented"
reflects the fact that, early on, educators were thinking of giftedness as encompassing
more than intellectual ability. In the 1950s, Paul Witty proposed that "any
child whose performance, in a potentially valuable line of human activity, is
consistently remarkable" is a gifted child. In 1972, the U.S. Office of
Education published a highly influential definition of giftedness that, in addition
to high general intellectual ability and high academic aptitude, listed leadership
ability, creative and productive thinking ability, ability in the visual and
performing arts, and psychomotor ability as co-equal forms of giftedness. And
in the late 1970s, Joseph Renzulli of the University of Connecticut proposed
a widely adopted definition of giftedness that contained three components: creativity,
task commitment, and above-average ability, the last term meant quite literally
as merely above-average ability, not Terman's 140 IQ, let alone Hollingworth's
180.
The practical consequence was, and is, that giftedness has many definitions
and that different school systems across the U.S. define giftedness in different,
sometimes radically different, ways. This means that a child can be "gifted"
in one school district and, upon moving to another community, find that he or
she is "not gifted" in the school district in which he or she is now
enrolled. Giftedness, in educational practice in the United States, is therefore
multiple, shifting, locally determined, and highly relative.
When one thinks about giftedness internationally and cross-culturally, the
conceptual difficulties are compounded. If, as I believe, giftedness is relative,
contingent on historical and cultural factors, and more a matter of values and
beliefs than scientific fact, giftedness will vary considerably in its meaning
and in its educational application from country to country. And, in fact, the
notion of gifted children may not make sense at all in some countries and cultures.
The danger of treating American thinking about giftedness as universal, a specific
manifestation of a more general national failing, is evident when one looks
at two features of definitions of giftedness that seem to be common to most
of the varied definitions in use today in the U.S. One of these is the idea
that, in the words of James J. Gallagher, "the ability to manipulate symbol
systems is the sine qua non of giftedness." It is true that in highly industrialized
countriesswheresliteracy is widespread, the ability to understand and express
oneself in symbolic media such as words, numbers, musical notation, visual representation,
and so forth is essential to success and to being recognized as excellent in
academic and other pursuits. However, there are cultures on this Earth in which
symbolic representation of knowledge has little importance compared to the ability
to orient oneself in space, to find sustenance, to negotiate social hierarchies,
and other abilities. In such cultures, something other than the ability to manipulate
symbol systems would necessarily be the sine qua non of giftedness.
A second feature seemingly common to American conceptions of giftedness is
the belief that giftedness is the result of a high level of ability within the
individual-a "gift"-and that one either has it or one does not, whether
at birth or as a result of environmental influences or a combination of the
two. There is reason to believe that this notion about giftedness may not be
universal. Research among Asian-American children and their families suggests
that for them giftedness is more likely to be seen as the result of hard work,
that one can be a gifted student if he or she values education sufficiently
and works hard enough.
It is simplistic and dangerous to treat all Asian-Americans with their varied
national and cultural backgrounds as a monolithic group, and it is similarly
dangerous to generalize from, say, Chinese-American students and families to
Chinese students and families, but there may be something to the idea that,
with a number of exceptions, there is a difference between Western and Eastern
conceptions of human ability. One of the wonderful things about teaching at
Teachers College, Columbia University, is the fact that we have so many international
students, including many from Asian countries. I have asked a number of my students
from China, Japan, Korea, and other countries, as well as a number of Asian-American
students, what their parents taught them about what people in my field call
"giftedness," and, on the basis of my far-from-scientific survey,
there does seem to be a greater tendency among Asian and Asian-American families
to stress to their children that academic excellence is more a matter of hard
work and dedication than of an immutable ability that has been given to one
as a gift.
The point that I am making, perhaps belaboring, is that giftedness is not a
fixed thing; that it is inevitably going to reflect the values and beliefs of
a community, a culture, a country; and that the practice of what we call "gifted
education" will, as a result, vary considerably from place to place. This
is what I mean by the "problem" of giftedness, a problem that can
only be solved in a culturally or nationally specific manner that reflects the
values and beliefs that inform the schools in a particular location.
Rationales for Gifted Education
Not only is the definition of giftedness likely to differ from country to country,
culture to culture, but the reasons for developing programs for gifted students,
however they are defined, are likely to vary as well. I have identified two
separate rationales for gifted education in the United States, and I think the
distinctions between them might have some relevance here. One rationale, which
I call the "national resources rationale," is based on the belief
that gifted students are a valuable national resource and that it is in the
interest of society at large to develop this resource. Giftedness in students
thus becomes linked to the potential for productivity and creativity in adulthood;
gifted students are defined as potentially gifted adults. The identification
of gifted students centers on identifying those students who have the traits
that suggest that they will contribute to the common good in the future.
A second rational, which I call the "special educational rationale,"
focuses not on the needs of the society or country but those of the individual.
Gifted students are seen as being exceptional, or different, because of their
abilities, so exceptional that they cannot receive the education to which they
are entitled unless their curriculum is in some way differentiated. Giftedness
is thus defined as significant variation from the norm, and identification focuses
on identifying exceptionality and educational need now, not the potential for
productivity in the future.
It is not difficult to see the degree to which values entersintosthe discussion
of these two bases for gifted education. The second rationale, the special educational
rationale, is clearly grounded in notions of individualism that may have little
currency in societies in which the common good is valued more than individual
needs. In the United States,swherespersonal autonomy is highly valued, some
would say over-valued, this rationale for gifted education is likely to be more
persuasive than it is in other countries,swheresthe needs of the individual
are subordinated to those of the family and the society at large.
Since each of these rationales has its own implications for practice. As I
suggested previously, each requires that giftedness be defined and identified
differently. Moreover, other rationales for gifted education could exist in
other countries and cultures. It is not difficult to see how, say, in a theocratic
society a completely different rationale for gifted education could emerge and
how differently giftedness would be defined and identified.
Again, the point is that giftedness, and thus educational practice related
to gifted children, is profoundly shaped by the society in which it is conceived.
Giftedness is local, not universal.
Curriculum Differentiation for Gifted Students
Thisshavingsbeen said, what general comments can one make about the schooling
of gifted students? If giftedness is so relative to time and place, are there
any generalities that have any validity? I think there are some, but they always
need to be considered within a local context.
First, research in the U.S., and to some extent in other countries, strongly
suggests that the most capable students do not develop their potential for outstanding
accomplishment unless some special provisions are made for them. A common curriculum
with the same content, the same forms of instruction, the same expectations
for growth for all students-in short a one-size-fits-all curriculum-will not
result in the development of the abilities of those students with the greatest
capacity for high performance. If curriculum and instruction are not differentiated,
if they are not modified to allow for the varying needs and abilities of different
students, students at the extremes of the ability distribution not be well served.
Students of lower ability will struggle to keep up with students of average
ability, and students of high ability will not be challenged, will not learn
much they do not already know, and very well might not develop their capacity
to think at higher, more productive levels. Ignoring the educational needs of
highly able students, assuming that they will develop on their own (which is
essentially to assume that schooling does not matter very much), inevitably
leads to talent wasted, which is a loss for the individual and the society.
Second, the development of talent and ability must be carried out in a value-consistent
context. By this, I mean that there needs to be consistency among the values
and goals of students, families, the peer culture, and school authorities. In
the U.S., attempts by educators to develop academic talent have sometimes been
hampered by a youth peer culture that values things other than scholarship.
Students who achieve in school sometimes risk social alienation unless their
academic success is "tempered" by such things as athletic prowess
or social facility. Some, not all, African-American students have been accused
of "acting white" when they have shown success in school. These are
characteristically American issues, but I think the underlying issue, that students
and their families need to share the goals and values of school officials if
talent-development efforts are to be successful, is a universal one. Changing
society through educational practice is an attractive idea, but effectively
using schools as an agent of societal change requires that students and their
families agree with , not merely acquiesce to, the goals and values of school
officials.
Third, no society can flourish without the optimal development of its most
talented citizens. I strongly believe that each child has an equal right to
an appropriate and effective education. But I also believe that it is essential
to the health and future of a society that it nourish and encourage the abilities
of those individuals, children and adults, who have special talents and abilities.
注1:James H. Borland is Professor of Education and Chair of the Department of Curriculum and Teaching at Teachers College, Columbia University,swhereshe also directs the graduate programs in the education of gifted students. Dr. Borland is the author of the book, Planning and Implementing Programs for the Gifted and numerous journal articles and book chapters, and he is editor of the forthcoming book, Rethinking Gifted Education. Dr. Borland is also editor of the Education and Psychology of the Gifted series of books from Teachers College Press and is past co-editor of the Section on Teaching, Learning, and Human Development of the American Educational Research Journal. He has lectured on the education of gifted students across the U.S. and abroad, and he has consulted with over 100 school districts, primarily as an evaluator of programs for gifted students. Dr. Borland has was awarded the Gifted Child Quarterly Paper of the Year Award for 1994 and 2000 and the Award for Excellence in Research from the Mensa Education and Research Foundation in 1989-1990 and in 1999-2000.
每月2元享用15M邮箱 中大奖游海南游韩国
|